[Dixielandjazz] Improvisation & Your Brain

GIORGI, RIC ricgiorgi at sympatico.ca
Mon Jun 6 07:39:18 PDT 2011


WOW, thank you Steve! A must read Great article. 

-----Original Message-----
From: dixielandjazz-bounces at ml.islandnet.com
[mailto:dixielandjazz-bounces at ml.islandnet.com] On Behalf Of Stephen G
Barbone
Sent: June-06-11 10:03 AM
To: Ric Giorgi
Cc: Dixieland Jazz Mailing List
Subject: [Dixielandjazz] Improvisation & Your Brain

Excerpted from a NY TIMES article.

When the Melody Takes a Detour, the Science Begins NY TIMES - By PAM BELLUCK
- June 6, 2011


In the middle of a World Science Festival panel on Saturday night, the
guitarist Pat Metheny took a sudden U-turn from the program he had planned.
Instead of performing one of his innovative compositions, plucked from any
of the phases of his career as a style-shifting jazz omnivore, Mr. Metheny,
performing with the bassist Larry Grenadier, decided on the spot to play a
jazz standard.

And not just any jazz standard, but an especially ubiquitous one:  
"Autumn Leaves."

His point, during this panel called "Music and the Spark of Spontaneity,"
was to illustrate what some of the scientists sharing the stage had been
talking about, that our brains have a kind of two- track approach to
deciding what we like in the world.

On the one hand, we are wired to respond to things that are familiar, to
predictability and patterns that help us make sense of what is around us.
But at the same time, too much familiarity breeds, if not contempt, at least
ennui or complacency. Our brains like newness too, things that surprise and
deviate from an expected pattern.

"I love the idea of this question of novelty versus familiarity," Mr.  
Metheny said. "'Autumn Leaves,' everybody knows that." And, he added, "for
the first few choruses, I'm going to use one finger on one string. I'm not
going to do anything that's more complicated than anyone who could play
simply would do."

Easy for him to say. Even the stripped-down version he started out with was
exceptionally musical, bending the familiar melody around Mr.  
Grenadier's exuberantly rhythmic bass. But as their performance became  
more intricate and adventurous, it underscored the science: They   
could travel miles from the melody, they could do calisthenics with the
chords, but the audience still understood it as "Autumn Leaves,"  
something they knew spiced with something entirely different.

Our preference for combining what we expect with what surprises us was
demonstrated in recent studies on what makes music expressive by Daniel J.
Levitin at McGill University, and also in brain imaging research by Edward
Large at Florida Atlantic University. Both scientists used classical music:
Chopin piano nocturnes or etudes in which the length and volume of notes
were adjusted to varying degrees.  
They found that musicians and nonmusicians alike responded most to versions
of the Chopin that included a lot of variety but not too much, and not
variety that was just thrown into the mix in a random, out-of-context way.

The World Science Festival panel in the Great Hall at Cooper Union focused
mostly on improvised music, especially the intuitive art of jazz, trying to
address the question of what is actually happening when a musician
spontaneously creates melodies, harmonies and rhythms that have never been
played before.

After "Autumn Leaves," the moderator, John Schaefer, the host of the
"Soundcheck" show on WNYC, gestured to the four scientists on the panel, and
said to Mr. Metheny, "Before I ask these guys what was going on in your
brain, let me ask you."

Mr. Metheny gave a thoughtful recitation of the elements in a jazz
musician's toolkit. "The harmony, the basic flow of the rhythms, the way the
chords are divided from key to key," he said, adding that "there's a whole
set of options" from which an improviser can choose, including playing
different musical scales or modes over a chord - "It could be Dorian, it
could be Mixolydian."

But then he Cheshire Catted it, saying, "but the real answer is I wasn't
thinking about any of them." Consider that "you just asked me a question in
perfect English," he said to Mr. Schaefer. "Did you think, 'O.K., I need a
verb?'" or "about how to hold your tongue?"

Mr. Metheny's answer pointed up another duality in the way our brains work,
that we have both conscious and unconscious brain processes, said one
panelist, Jamshed Bharucha, a neuroscientist and the incoming president of
Cooper Union, who is also a violinist. "The vast majority of stuff that goes
on in our brain we do not have conscious access to," he said. "It's
automatic."

But music requires "years and years of practice in order to make what is
conscious unconscious," he said. Plus, improvisation is not just free-form
playing - there has to be a mastery of structure and discipline. "If you
want to fly off the edge of a cliff, you have to know where the cliff is,"
he said.

Improvising, in fact, may be one of the most complex abilities humans can
develop because "it requires you to perfect all these different skills,"
said Gary Marcus, a psychology professor at New York University. Among other
things, he said, improvising requires "terrific alignment between your ears
and hands"; some learned or instinctive understanding of music theory;
empathy, because improvising usually involves interacting with and
responding to other musicians; and "fantastic motor control - you need to be
both fast and accurate, and the brain's natural tendency is to be either
fast or accurate."

Unraveling improvisation is, not surprisingly, a little like trying to
capture a unicorn. And some people might wonder if there's a risk that
scientific investigation could result in something resembling a scene in the
Tom Stoppard play "Rosencrantz and Guildenstern Are Dead," in which, as more
and more people are able to see a unicorn, they perceive it as something
pedestrian and uninspiring: "a horse with an arrow in its forehead."

At one point, Mr. Schaefer asked, "Is it worth the effort to try and
demystify something that we enjoy and that we like to think of as kind of
mysterious and magical?"

Charles Limb, a surgeon and specialist in the neurology of the ear at Johns
Hopkins School of Medicine, who also plays saxophone, said, "I don't think
in any way that music needs science," but "music is a tool by which we can
understand the brain" and the science of music "really teaches us something
fundamental about who we are, why we're here."

Dr. Limb has been putting jazz musicians into a functional magnetic
resonance imaging machine, a process which requires them to lie on their
backs, slide into the brain scanner, and keep their heads perfectly still,
while their hands, stretched out in front of them outside the scanner, play
on a jury-rigged piano keyboard. The musicians' brain activity is monitored
as they played a written-down jazz melody they were asked to memorize and
then as they improvised to the chord changes of that melody.

When the musicians improvised, Dr. Limb found, areas of the brain's
prefrontal cortex linked to self-expression were activated, but an area
linked to inhibition and self-monitoring "kind of shuts down when you go
creative," he said. That did not happen when musicians played a memorized
piece.

Dr. Limb is also putting hip-hop artists into the brain scanner.  
"They're totally into it," he said. "They say, 'You know I've wanted to know
what's going on in my head for the past 20 years.'" He asks them to recite a
memorized rap and then to improvise lyrics using certain cued words.
Preliminary results suggest that rap improvisation activates different areas
from music improvisation.

Aaron Berkowitz, who has researched the neural basis of improvisation and is
also a pianist and fortepianist, used a different approach to compare the
brains of musicians and nonmusicians when they created five-note melodies.
He found that the amount of novelty in the melodies was the same for
musicians and nonmusicians, and that musicians were not activating more
music-related areas of the brain.  
But he found that "musicians were turning off a part of the brain,"  
involved in "a special type of attention," he said. It was a different area
from what Dr. Limb was studying, but the implication is similar.  
The fact that this area gets inhibited when musicians play enables the
performers to tune out a cellphone ring in the audience or noise from a
malfunctioning amplifier, Dr. Berkowitz said.

Asked how, when performing music, he balances the ideas and feelings in his
head with the external stimuli of the audience, the place he is performing,
and extraneous sounds, Mr. Metheny explained his approach.  
He respects and appreciates the audience, but "I'm playing for myself -
anything other than that would be a guess," he said. "If you start worrying
about what critics say, or a record company, or the audience, then you get
paralyzed. The only thing I know for sure is what I love."

Mr. Metheny said that when he plays, "to a certain degree I remain somewhat
detached emotionally; I'm kind of listening" and thinking at various points,
"if I was listening to this, which I am, what would I like the guitar player
to play next? And I would do it, or sometimes I would do the opposite. The
best musicians are not the best players, they're the best listeners."

But even Mr. Metheny's decades of experience doesn't give him the ability to
always play the notes he wants to. "You've told me that actually the music
coming out of the guitar is not nearly as good as what you're hearing in
your head," Dr. Limb said.

"It's actually quite far away from that," Mr. Metheny said.

Dr. Marcus, who is not a musician but took up the guitar two years ago, said
he realized how liberating it was to make up music and that improvising
"tells us a lot about how we learn things and what we can get good at." He
displayed an app he created that allows people to mix and match musical
elements as a kind of half-machine, half-man method of improvising and
composing.

"One of the things you start to realize is that anything starts to sound
more musical when you hear it again," he said. The mixture of consistency
and variability, like having a steady beat but changing the melody, he said,
is why "music is as powerful as it is." . . .

Maybe improvisation is not just the culmination of many learned abilities,
but a sophisticated coordination of skills that reflects something critical
about the way human brains work, the scientists and musician seemed to be
saying.

"Lately," Mr. Metheny said, "I've been thinking about jazz not so much as a
destination but as a process, but even more than that as a symptom."
_______________________________________________
To unsubscribe or change your e-mail preferences for the Dixieland Jazz
Mailing list, or to find the online archives, please visit:

http://ml.islandnet.com/mailman/listinfo/dixielandjazz



Dixielandjazz mailing list
Dixielandjazz at ml.islandnet.com

-----
No virus found in this message.
Checked by AVG - www.avg.com
Version: 10.0.1382 / Virus Database: 1511/3684 - Release Date: 06/06/11




More information about the Dixielandjazz mailing list