[Dixielandjazz] This is your brain on music

Steve Barbone barbonestreet at earthlink.net
Sun Dec 31 13:05:42 PST 2006


This is a very long article. However it is also very interesting and will,
if you read it, take away some of the myths about music and how the human
brain hears it. And how we are ALL wired to music from birth.

You may find the segment about the Beetles demo record interesting. About
half way through.

Cheers,
Steve Barbone

Music of the Hemispheres

NY Times - By CLIVE THOMPSON - December 31, 2006

Daniel Levitin is the rare music scientist to have worked in the music
business. ³Pop musicians compose with timbre,² he said. ³Pitch and harmony
are becoming less important.²

³Listen to this,² Daniel Levitin said. ³What is it?² He hit a button on his
computer keyboard and out came a half-second clip of music. It was just two
notes blasted on a raspy electric guitar, but I could immediately identify
it: the opening lick to the Rolling Stones¹ ³Brown Sugar.²

Then he played another, even shorter snippet: a single chord struck once on
piano. Again I could instantly figure out what it was: the first note in
Elton John¹s live version of ³Benny and the Jets.²

Dr. Levitin beamed. ³You hear only one note, and you already know who it
is,² he said. ³So what I want to know is: How we do this? Why are we so good
at recognizing music?²

This is not merely some whoa-dude epiphany that a music fan might have while
listening to a radio contest. Dr. Levitin has devoted his career to
exploring this question. He is a cognitive psychologist who runs the
Laboratory for Music Perception, Cognition and Expertise at McGill
University in Montreal, perhaps the world¹s leading lab in probing why music
has such an intense effect on us.

³By the age of 5 we are all musical experts, so this stuff is clearly wired
really deeply into us,² said Dr. Levitin, an eerily youthful-looking 49,
surrounded by the pianos, guitars and enormous 16-track mixers that make his
lab look more like a recording studio.

This summer he published ³This Is Your Brain on Music² (Dutton), a
layperson¹s guide to the emerging neuroscience of music. Dr. Levitin is an
unusually deft interpreter, full of striking scientific trivia. For example
we learn that babies begin life with synesthesia, the trippy confusion that
makes people experience sounds as smells or tastes as colors. Or that the
cerebellum, a part of the brain that helps govern movement, is also wired to
the ears and produces some of our emotional responses to music. His
experiments have even suggested that watching a musician perform affects
brain chemistry differently from listening to a recording.

Dr. Levitin is singular among music scientists for actually having come out
of the music industry. Before getting his Ph.D. he spent 15 years as a
record producer, working with artists ranging from the Blue Öyster Cult to
Chris Isaak. While still in graduate school he helped Stevie Wonder assemble
a best-of collection; in 1992 Dr. Levitin¹s sensitive ears detected that MCA
Records had accidentally used third-generation backup tapes to produce seven
Steely Dan CDs, and he embarrassed the label by disclosing it in Billboard
magazine. He has earned nine gold and platinum albums, which he tucks in
corners of his lab, office and basement at home. ³They look a little scary
when you put them all in one place, so I spread them around,² he said.

Martin Grant, the dean of science at McGill, compares Dr. Levitin¹s split
professional personality to that of Brian Greene, the pioneering
string-theory scientist who also writes mass-market books. ³Some people are
good popularizers, and some are good scientists, but not usually both at
once,² Dr. Grant said. ³Dan¹s actually cutting edge in his field.²

Scientifically, Dr. Levitin¹s colleagues credit him for focusing attention
on how music affects our emotions, turf that wasn¹t often covered by
previous generations of psychoacousticians, who studied narrower questions
about how the brain perceives musical sounds. ³The questions he asks are
very very musical, very concerned with the fact that music is an art that we
interact with, not just a bunch of noises,² said Rita Aiello, an adjunct
professor in the department of psychology at New York University.

Ultimately, scientists say, his work offers a new way to unlock the
mysteries of the brain: how memory works, how people with autism think, why
our ancestors first picked up instruments and began to play, tens of
thousands of years ago.

DR. LEVITIN originally became interested in producing in 1981, when his band
‹ a punk outfit called the Mortals ‹ went into the recording studio. None of
the other members were interested in the process, so he made all the
decisions behind the board. ³I actually became a producer because I saw the
producers getting all the babes,² he said. ³They were stealing them from the
guitarists.² He dropped out of college to work with alternative bands.

Producers, he noted, were able to notice impossibly fine gradations of
quality in music. Many could identify by ear the type of amplifiers and
recording tape used on an album.

³So I started wondering: How was the brain able to do this?² Dr. Levitin
said. ³What¹s going on there, and why are some people better than others?
And why is music such an emotional experience?² He began sitting in on
neuroscience classes at Stanford University.

³Even back then, Dan was never satisfied with the simple answer,² said Howie
Klein, a former president of Reprise and Sire Records. ³He was always poking
and prodding.²

By the ¹90s Dr. Levitin was disenchanted with the music industry. ³When
they¹re dropping Van Morrison and Elvis Costello because they don¹t sell
enough records,² he said, ³I knew it was time to move on.² Academic friends
persuaded him to pursue a science degree. They bet that he would have good
intuitions on how to design music experiments.

They were right. Traditionally music psychologists relied on ³simple
melodies they¹d written themselves,² Dr. Levitin said. What could that tell
anyone about the true impact of powerful music?

For his first experiment he came up with an elegant concept: He stopped
people on the street and asked them to sing, entirely from memory, one of
their favorite hit songs. The results were astonishingly accurate. Most
people could hit the tempo of the original song within a four-percent margin
of error, and two-thirds sang within a semitone of the original pitch, a
level of accuracy that wouldn¹t embarrass a pro.

³When you played the recording of them singing alongside the actual
recording of the original song, it sounded like they were singing along,²
Dr. Levitin said.

It was a remarkable feat. Most memories degrade and distort with time; why
would pop music memories be so sharply encoded? Perhaps because music
triggers the reward centers in our brains. In a study published last year
Dr. Levitin and group of neuroscientists mapped out precisely how.

Observing 13 subjects who listened to classical music while in an M.R.I.
machine, the scientists found a cascade of brain-chemical activity. First
the music triggered the forebrain, as it analyzed the structure and meaning
of the tune. Then the nucleus accumbus and ventral tegmental area activated
to release dopamine, a chemical that triggers the brain¹s sense of reward.

The cerebellum, an area normally associated with physical movement, reacted
too, responding to what Dr. Levitin suspected was the brain¹s predictions of
where the song was going to go. As the brain internalizes the tempo, rhythm
and emotional peaks of a song, the cerebellum begins reacting every time the
song produces tension (that is, subtle deviations from its normal melody or
tempo).

³When we saw all this activity going on precisely in sync, in this order, we
knew we had the smoking gun,² he said. ³We¹ve always known that music is
good for improving your mood. But this showed precisely how it happens.²

The subtlest reason that pop music is so flavorful to our brains is that it
relies so strongly on timbre. Timbre is a peculiar blend of tones in any
sound; it is why a tuba sounds so different from a flute even when they are
playing the same melody in the same key. Popular performers or groups, Dr.
Levitin argued, are pleasing not because of any particular virtuosity, but
because they create an overall timbre that remains consistent from song to
song. That quality explains why, for example, I could identify even a single
note of Elton John¹s ³Benny and the Jets.²

³Nobody else¹s piano sounds quite like that,² he said, referring to Mr.
John. ³Pop musicians compose with timbre. Pitch and harmony are becoming
less important.²

Dr. Levitin dragged me over to a lab computer to show me what he was talking
about. ³Listen to this,² he said, and played an MP3. It was pretty awful: a
poorly recorded, nasal-sounding British band performing, for some reason, a
Spanish-themed ballad.

Dr. Levitin grinned. ³That,² he said, ³is the original demo tape of the
Beatles. It was rejected by every record company. And you can see why. To
you and me it sounds terrible. But George Martin heard this and thought, ŒOh
yeah, I can imagine a multibillion-dollar industry built on this.¹

³Now that¹s musical genius.²

THE largest audience that Dr. Levitin has performed in front of was 1,000
people, when he played backup saxophone for Mel Tormé. Years of being
onstage piqued Dr. Levitin¹s interest in another aspect of musical
experience: watching bands perform. Does the brain experience a live
performance differently from a recorded one?

To find out, he and Bradley Vines, a graduate student, devised an
interesting experiment. They took two clarinet performances and played them
for three groups of listeners: one that heard audio only; one that saw a
video only; and one that had audio and video. As each group listened,
participants used a slider to indicate how their level of tension was rising
or falling.

One rapid, complex passage caused tension in all groups, but less in the one
watching and listening simultaneously. Why? Possibly, Dr. Levitin said,
because of the performer¹s body language: the clarinetist appeared to be
relaxed even during that rapid-fire passage, and the audience picked up on
his visual cues. The reverse was also true: when the clarinetist played in a
subdued way but appeared animated, the people with only video felt more
tension than those with only audio.

In another, similar experiment the clarinetist fell silent for a few bars.
This time the viewers watching the video maintained a higher level of
excitement because they could see that he was gearing up to launch into a
new passage. The audio-only listeners had no such visual cues, and they
regarded the silence as much less exciting.

This spring Dr. Levitin began an even more involved experiment to determine
how much emotion is conveyed by live performers. In April he took
participants in a Boston Symphony Orchestra concert ‹ the conductor Keith
Lockhart, five of the musicians and 15 audience members ‹ and wired them
with sensors to measure their state of arousal, including heart rate, body
movements and muscle tension.

At one point during the performance Mr. Lockhart swung his wrist with such
force that a sensor attached to his cuff went flying off. Dr. Levitin¹s team
tried to reattach it with duct tape, until the conductor objected ‹ ³Did you
just put duct tape on an Armani?² he asked ‹ and lighter surgical tape was
used instead.

The point of the experiment is to determine whether the conductor creates
noticeable changes in the emotional tenor of the performance. Dr. Levitin
says he suspects there¹s a domino effect: the conductor becomes particularly
animated, transmits this to the orchestra and then to the audience, in a
matter of seconds. Mr. Lockhart is skeptical. ³As a conductor,² he said,
³I¹m a causatory force for music, but I¹m not a causatory force for
emotion.² But Dr. Levitin is still crunching the data.

³It might not turn out to be like that,² he said, ³But wouldn¹t it be cool
if it did?² 

Dr. Levitin¹s work has occasionally undermined some cherished beliefs about
music. For example recent years have seen an explosion of ³Baby Mozart²
videos and toys, based on the idea ‹ popular since the ¹80s ‹ that musical
and mathematical ability are inherently linked.

But Dr. Levitin argued that this could not be true, based on his study of
people with Williams syndrome, a genetic disorder that leaves people with
low intelligence. Their peak mental capacities are typically those of young
child, with no ability to calculate quantities. Dr. Levitin once asked a
woman with Williams to hold up her hand for five seconds; she left it in the
air for a minute and a half. ³No concept of time at all,² he said, ³and
definitely no math.²

Yet people with Williams possess unusually high levels of musical ability.
One Williams boy Dr. Levitin met was so poorly coordinated he could not open
the case to his clarinet. But once he was holding the instrument, his
coordination problems vanished, and he could play fluidly. Music cannot be
indispensably correlated with math, Dr. Levitin noted, if Williams people
can play music. He is now working on a study that compares autistics ‹ some
of whom have excellent mathematical ability, but little musical ability ‹ to
people with Williams; in the long run, he said, he thinks it could help shed
light on why autistic brains develop so differently.

Not all of Dr. Levitin¹s idea have been easily accepted. He argues, for
example, that music is an evolutionary adaptation: something that men
developed as a way to demonstrate reproductive fitness. (Before you laugh,
consider the sex lives of today¹s male rock stars.) Music also helped social
groups cohere. ³Music has got to be useful for survival, or we would have
gotten rid of it years ago,² he said.

But Steven Pinker, a cognitive scientist at Harvard known for his defense of
evolutionary psychology, has publicly disparaged this idea. Dr. Pinker has
called music ³auditory cheesecake,² something pleasant but not
evolutionarily nutritious. If it is a sexual signal for reproduction, then
why, Dr. Pinker asked, does ³a 60-year-old woman enjoy listening to
classical music when she¹s alone at home?² Dr. Levitin wrote an entire
chapter refuting Dr. Pinker¹s arguments; when I asked Dr. Pinker about Dr.
Levitin¹s book he said he hadn¹t read it.

Nonetheless Dr. Levitin plugs on, and sometimes still plugs in. He continues
to perform music, doing several gigs a year with Diminished Faculties, a
ragtag band composed entirely of professors and students at McGill. On a
recent December afternoon members assembled in a campus ballroom to do a
sound check for their performance that evening at a holiday party. Playing a
blue Stratocaster, Dr. Levitin crooned the Chris Isaak song ³Wicked Game.²
³I¹m not a great guitarist, and I¹m not a great singer,² he said.

But he is not bad, either, and still has those producer¹s ears. When ³Wicked
Game² ended, the bass player began noodling idly, playing the first few
notes of a song that seemed instantly familiar to all the younger students
gathered. ³That¹s Nirvana, right?² Dr. Levitin said, cocking his head and
squinting. ³ ŒCome As You Are.¹ I love that song.²





More information about the Dixielandjazz mailing list